Document Type : Review papers
Authors
1 Psychology, Faculty of Arts and Humanities, The British University in Egypt, Cairo, Egypt
2 Km 8, Misr Ismailia Desert Road 5th District
Abstract
Keywords
Main Subjects
Adaptability is born out of the ability to produce non-automatic responses in face of a world of unpredictability and complexity (Barkley, 2001; Hawkins & Blakeslee, 2007). Fundamentally, executive functions are perceived to embody adaptation and learning. Although commonly referred to as cognitive tools, executive functions could be explained as the inherent drive to harmonize internal models with external realities (Pezzulo, 2012). Through planning, anticipating, and reflecting, one can attain perseverance and a sense of integrity and direction. Recent research has commenced towards explaining executive functions through new lenses. Traditionalistic views asserted that the brain merely reacts to upcoming stimuli; however, prevailing views contend that the brain is in a dynamic cycle where it plans, anticipates and predicts upcoming stimuli and actively updates any predictive errors to enable behaviour with intention and purpose (Kopp, 2012; Parr et al., 2023; Pezzulo, 2012).
There is no doubt that altered executive functioning could lead to disrupted self- governance (Barkley, 2001). Research in psychology and psychiatry have emphasized the importance of studying changes in executive function and cognitive control in psychiatric disorders. Prominently, results have shown compromised abilities in accurate decision making (Cáceda et al., 2014; Scholl & Klein-Flügge, 2017); a dissonance between desired goals and actions taken (Gillan et al., 2015; Gillan et al., 2016; Griffiths et al., 2014; Voon et al., 2017); and inability to produce accurate predictions and update internal models (Doll et al., 2012; Gradin et al., 2011; Stephan & Mathys, 2014).
Research postulates that obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and anorexia nervosa (AN) might share similar cognitive dysfunctions and neural mechanisms. With results showing high comorbidity between these disorders (Drakes et al., 2021; Mandelli et al., 2020; Pollack & Forbush, 2013); shared symptomatology (Altman & Shankman, 2009; Latif & Moulding, 2024; Williams et al., 2022); and genetic relatedness (Yilmaz et al., 2018), it is intuitive to see whether they exhibit similar executive functions and neural mechanisms. OCD is popularly characterised through the well-known ‘OCD Loop’, in which unwanted and intrusive obsessions give rise to anxious feelings that could only be relieved by performing ritual habits or compulsions, and the cycle perpetuates (Laposa et al., 2018; Robbins et al., 2011; Salkovskis, 1985; Taylor et al., 2007). Similarly, anorexia nervosa has shown to exhibit a similar cycle. Obsessions and unwanted negative thoughts about weight, appearance and food rises anxiety levels, which inevitably leads to compulsive actions such as body checking, purging and restricting (Matsunaga et al., 1999; Shafran, 2002; Sherman et al., 2006). Likewise, these compulsions only lead to short-lived relief. Here, one can observe a profound struggle with the very essence of executive control. A shift from conscious deliberation of aligning goals with actions to a manifestation of routines and automated responses is seen.
Changes in executive control in patients with AN or OCD is observed throughout the literature. For instance, a large sample of patients with AN were assessed using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task and were compared to healthy participants (Tchanturia et al., 2012). Results revealed that patients faced difficulty and performed worse than healthy controls. High preservative errors in both patients with active illness and recovered patients attests to the notion that eating disorders have impaired executive functioning, specifically cognitive flexibility and
inhibition of prepotent responses (Tchanturia et al., 2012). Similar conclusions were found when comparing the performance of OCD patients and healthy controls in a variety of executive functioning tasks such as go/no-go task, stop-signal task, Wisconsin Card Sorting task (WCST), and n-back task (Martínez-Esparza et al., 2021; Youssef et al., 2020). Sternheim et al.’s (2022) study has highlighted the importance of self-reported measures alongside executive function tasks, explaining that both AN and OCD patients self-reported more inflexibility did not link to their actual performance.
Despite such evidence, opposing results were identified. Although both patient groups present with executive deficits, neuroimaging outcomes differed. To explicate, Thomas et al.’s (2022) review has concluded that a divergence between behavioural performance and neural activity between AN and OCD patients. In other words, even if both patient groups present with similar executive deficits in cognitive-behavioural tasks, the underlying neural mechanism functions differently. This was further supported by Li et al. (2023), in which poor performance in inhibitory tasks was not linked to similar brain activity in eating disorders and OCD. Evidence from previous reviews has mainly centred on domain-based analysis such as inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and working memory. However, much of the research have used different executive function tasks to assess the same domain. This could lead to confusions due to (1) different operationalizations of the same domain, (2) tasks may engage in selective and specific cognitive processes, and (3) the same task can measure many domains, making it difficult to isolate one domain to a specific task. The argument of whether to measure executive function as domain- general or domain-specific processes or to assess it through domain-based or task-based analysis has been discussed in past research (for a deeper read, see Assem et al., 2024; Chan et al., 2007; Jurado & Rosselli, 2007; Löffler et al., 2024). Therefore, the following review will aim to synthesize research that assessed executive function in OCD and AN using a task-based approach.
Literature Search and Study Selection
This systematic review was carried out using the PRISMA 2020 Guidelines (Page et al., 2021). Both Scopus and PubMed were used pinpoint functional magnetic resonance imaging studies examining executive function in OCD and AN until 17th November 2024. The search strategy consisted of five items: population (OCD or AN); fMRI imaging; executive function domains; and executive functioning tasks. The full detailed search strategy can be found in the Appendix. After duplicates were removed, M.S and A.A both independently screened the titles and abstracts of the remaining studies and assessed against the eligibility criteria. Studies that coincided with the eligibility criteria were taken for full-text examination. Any disagreements were resolved through discussions and reaching consensus.
All chosen studies were divided into two folders and were uploaded on Zotero (a reference manager software). Data extraction was performed independently by each reviewer; however, all information was uploaded on a shared Excel spreadsheet for cross-validation. Important information that was looked for were: study characteristics (e.g. authors and year of publication); sample characteristics (e.g. age); fMRI analysis and executive function paradigm used; and case- control contrasts. Full study details can be found in Tables 1 and 2 (in the Appendix).
Quality assessment was conducted for the included articles using the Standard Quality Assessment Criteria (SQAC; Kmet et al., 2004; Wollesen et al., 2019). Quality assessment was based on the following criteria: (1) question and objective of study is sufficiently described, (2) study design is identified and appropriate to study’s research question, (3) information on sample selection, input variables, and inclusion/exclusion criteria are described and appropriate, (4) sufficient description of sample demographic characteristics are given, (5) outcome and exposure measures are precisely described and defined, (6) study’s methodological analyses are described and appropriate, (7) reports of estimates of variance in results are provided, (8) results are reported sufficiently in detail, (9) all conclusions are supported by the data. Points for each criterion were as follows: 2 points if criteria are fulfilled, 1 point if criteria are partially fulfilled, 0 points if criteria are not fulfilled. Total quality score is out of 18, and a minimum score of 14 (75-77%) is required for a study to be considered a high-quality study.
Study Selection
A total of 332 studies were obtained in the literature search: 86 studies from PubMed and 238 studies from Scopus. With regards to research that included OCD patients, a total of 230 studies were obtained (n = 64 PubMed; n = 166 Scopus). After excluding 105 studies due to inappositeness and 54 studies were removed due to duplication, 71 studies were chosen for full- text examination. Out of the 71, 43 studies were excluded for reasons such as: EF tasks were done outside the fMRI scanner; fMRI results were reported post-treatment; EF tasks included an emotional stimulus; same sample datasets were used. Moreover, 3 studies were found from previous literature reviews (Gruner & Pittenger, 2016; Nakao et al., 2014; Saxena et al., 2009).
Therefore, a total of 31 fMRI studies that were concerned with executive function tasks in OCD patients were selected for this review.
With regards to research that included AN patients, a total of 94 studies were obtained (n
= 22 PubMed; n = 72 Scopus). A total of 22 studies were chosen for full-text examination after 62 incompatible studies and 10 duplicates were excluded. After full-text examination, 13 studies were excluded due to the following reasons: participants were stress-induced; participants had to perform physical activity; participants were either hungry or satiated during fMRI scanning; food and affective stimuli was included; EF tasks were taken outside fMRI. Backward literature search extracted 4 additional studies (Fuglset et al., 2016; Kappou et al., 2021). This resulted in a total of 13 fMRI studies that analysed executive function tasks in AN patients included in this review.
High quality scores (threshold of >=14, 75-77%) were given to 37 studies. The remaining 6 studies received a good quality score, with scores ranging from 12-13 (66-72%), and thus were not excluded from the study. Overview of the scores can be found in the Appendix.
The final 43 chosen studies consisted of 2,068 participants, with sample sizes varying from 15 to 100. Thirty-one of OCD-focused studies and seven of AN-focused studies included adult samples (ages ranging from 21 to 51), with three OCD-focused studies and six AN-focused studies included adolescent samples (ages ranging from 13 to 18).
The studies addressing OCD examined the following executive functioning tasks: Go/No- Go (n = 7), N-back (n = 6), Task-Switch (n = 4), Stroop (n = 4), Tower of London (n = 3). Other tasks included Stop-Signal (n = 3), Simon Task (n = 1), Flanker Task (n = 2), Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST, n = 2), Item-Recognition Working Memory Task (n = 1), and Imagination, Suppression and Erase Task (n = 1).
In terms of AN-targeted studies, most common executive functioning tasks used were the Go/No-Go Task (n = 3) and the WCST (n = 3). Other tasks included Set-Shift (n = 2), Stop-Signal (n = 1), Target-Detection Task (n = 1), N-back (n = 1), Implicit Sequence Learning Task (n = 1), Embedded Figures Task (n = 1).
While almost all studies have applied general linear modeling (GLM), psychophysiological interactions (PPI; n = 7) and functional connectivity (FC; n = 4) have been used. Whole-brain analysis was conducted in 89% of studies (n = 27 OCD-focused studies; n = 13 AN-focused studies), whilst 69% of studies used region of interest analysis (ROI; n = 23 OCD-focused studies; n = 8 AN-focused studies).
Brain regions that were reported in at least 50% of the studies were taken into consideration. In the following results, numbers in brackets refer to subregions. Subregions were counted by each instance they were reported, acknowledging that the same subregion has been reported by more than one study. Twenty out of 32 OCD-specific studies have reported more activation in healthy controls compared to OCD in frontal (n = 26), cingulate (n = 20), temporal
(n = 20), sensorimotor (n = 15), subcortical and limbic regions (n = 21) (Cocchi et al., 2011; De Wit et al., 2012; Fitzgerald et al., 2005; Gu et al., 2007; Han et al., 2011; Heinzel et al., 2017; Kang et al., 2012; Koch et al., 2012; Koçak et al., 2011; Masharipov et al., 2023; Morein-Zamir et al., 2015; Page et al., 2009; Remijnse et al., 2013; Roth et al., 2007; Schlösser et al., 2010; Thorsen et al., 2020; Tolin et al., 2013; Vaghi et al., 2017; Van Den Heuvel et al., 2005).
Twenty-three out of 32 OCD-focused studies have reported more activation in OCD compared to HC in frontal (n = 29), temporal (n = 13), parietal (n = 17), and sensorimotor regions (n = 19) (De Wit et al., 2012; Fitzgerald et al., 2005; Fuglset et al., 2016; Gruner & Pittenger, 2016; Gu et al., 2007; Han et al., 2011; Heinzel et al., 2017; Henseler et al., 2008; Hough et al., 2016; Kang et al., 2012; Kappou et al., 2021; Kim et al., 2021; Koçak et al., 2011; Masharipov et al., 2023; Morein-Zamir et al., 2015; Nakao et al., 2008; Page et al., 2009; Remijnse et al., 2013; Roth et al., 2007; Schlösser et al., 2010; Thorsen et al., 2020; Tolin et al., 2013; Vaghi et al., 2017; Vanderwee et al., 2003).
Seven out of 13 AN-focused studies reported more activation in HC compared to AN in frontal (n = 6), cingulate (n = 4), sensorimotor (n = 5), occipital (n = 7), subcortical (n = 5), insula and opercular regions (n = 4; Castro-Fornieles et al., 2019; Sato et al., 2013; Suttkus et al., 2021; Wierenga et al., 2014; Zastrow et al., 2009).
Seven out of 13 AN-focused studies reported more activation in AN compared to HC in prefrontal (n = 5), parietal (n = 4), and occipital regions (n = 9) (Castro-Fornieles et al., 2019; Lao- Kaim et al., 2015; Lock et al., 2010; Noda et al., 2021; Van Autreve et al., 2016; Wierenga et al., 2014).
Figures 1 and 2 show common brain area activation patterns reported by most studies (refer to the Appendix). In OCD, hyperactivation was seen in the following brain areas: middle frontal gyrus (MFG); inferior frontal gyrus (IFG); precentral gyrus (PRG); postcentral gyrus (PoCG); inferior parietal lobule (IPL); supramarginal gyrus (SMG); and pre-supplementary motor area (pre- SMA; Cocchi et al., 2011; De Vries et al., 2013; De Wit et al., 2012; Heinzel et al., 2017; Hough et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2021; Meram et al., 2020; Morein-Zamir et al., 2015; Remijnse et al., 2013; Schlösser et al., 2010). Hypoactivation was shown in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and premotor cortex (PM; Gu et al., 2007; Koçak et al., 2011; Page et al., 2009; Van Den Heuvel et al., 2005).
Conflicting results were shown in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), superior frontal gyrus (SFG), superior temporal gyrus (STG), middle temporal gyrus (MTG), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), supplementary motor area (SMA), precuneus (PRCU), and cerebellum (Cocchi et al., 2011; De Vries et al., 2013; Fitzgerald et al., 2005; Gu et al., 2007; Han et al., 2011; Heinzel et al., 2017; Hough et al., 2016; Kang et al., 2012; Kim et al., 2021; Koch et al., 2012; Koçak et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2023; Maltby et al., 2004; Masharipov et al., 2023; Meram et al., 2020; Morein-Zamir et al., 2015; Nakao et al., 2008; Page et al., 2009; Remijnse et al., 2013; Roth et al., 2007; Schlösser et al., 2010; Thorsen et al., 2020; Tolin et al., 2013; Van Den Heuvel et al., 2005).
In AN, increased brain activation was reported in the PRCU (Lao-Kaim et al., 2015; Van Autreve et al., 2016; Noda et al., 2021), and cuneus (Castro-Fornieles et al., 2019; Noda et al., 2021). Decreased brain activation was shown in the IFG, MFG, PRG, cerebellum, and lingual gyrus (LG; Castro-Fornieles et al., 2019; Sato et al., 2013; Wierenga et al., 2014; Zastrow et al.,
2009). Conflicting results were seen in the ACC (Lock et al., 2010; Wierenga et al., 2014; Zastrow et al., 2009). Both OCD and AN have presented increased overall activation of the insula during executive functioning tasks (Heinzel et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2021; Nakao et al., 2008; Roth et al., 2007; Tolin et al., 2013; Van Autreve et al., 2016).
Seven studies have used the go/no-go task to measure executive functioning in OCD. Out of these studies, the most reported brain regions were the ACC (n = 5), thalamus (n = 4), caudate (n = 4). Other brain areas included the posterior cingulate (n = 3), precuneus (n = 3), insula (n = 2), and middle temporal gyrus (n = 2). Three studies have reported more activation in OCD patients compared to healthy controls in the ACC (Maltby et al., 2004; Morein-Zamir et al., 2015; Tolin et al., 2013); whereas two studies have reported less activation (Masharipov et al., 2023; Page et al., 2009). This was also shown in the caudate nucleus (CN), in which two studies have reported hyperactivity in OCD (Masharipov et al., 2023; Roth et al., 2007) and two other studies have reported hypoactivity in OCD (Masharipov et al., 2023; Page et al., 2009). Of the studies that reported activation in the thalamus, two have reported more activation in healthy controls compared to OCD patients (Masharipov et al., 2023; Page et al., 2009), and only one study has reported variable activity levels (Morein-Zamir et al., 2015).
In terms of research pertaining to AN patients, three studies have applied the go/no-go task and have reported distinct brain activations. Two studies have identified heightened activity compared to healthy controls and one study has reported the contrary. For instance, Lock et al. (2010) have reported elevated activity in AN (binge-eating type) compared to HC in the following brain areas: PRG, ACC, MTG, STG, hypothalamus, and DLPFC. On the other hand, heightened activity in the cuneus and precuneus were reported by Noda et al. (2021). With regards to low- level activation compared to HC, the amygdala and hippocampus were reported (Suttkus et al., 2021).
Taking a closer look at between-group comparisons, similar brain regions but with distinct neural activity is shown. These include the following: MTG, ACC, PRCU, PoCG, CN, LG (Lock et al., 2010; Maltby et al., 2004; Masharipov et al., 2023; Morein-Zamir et al., 2015; Noda et al., 2021; Page et al., 2009; Roth et al., 2007; Tolin et al., 2013).
Overall, only two studies addressing OCD have assessed executive functioning through the Stroop task. There was no consensus on brain regions reported. Through ROI analysis, Schlösser et al. (2010) reported elevated activity in OCD in the dACC, DLPFC, PRG, SFG, MFG, SPL, and IPL, and reduced activity levels in the occipital lobe. In contrast, Page et al. (2009) reported higher activity in posterior cingulate gyrus (PCG) and cerebellum, and reduced activity in the precuneus, MTG, STG, STJ, IPJ, and SPL.
Three studies (one used ROI analysis and the other whole-brain analysis) presented distinct neural activity in OCD patients during stop-signal tasks. Essentially, de Wit et al.’s (2012) findings suggest that OCD patients exhibit higher brain activity in the pre-SMA compared to controls, and
lower brain activity in the IFG and inferior parietal cortex (IPC). Through whole-brain analysis, brain regions that exhibit greater activation in OCD include the parahippocampal gyrus (PHG), SPC, and cerebellum (Kang et al., 2012). Moreover, healthy controls showed greater neural activations compared to patients in the middle temporal cortex (MTC), middle occipital cortex (MOC), PRG, superior temporal cortex (STC), ACC, CN, putamen, fusiform face area (FFC), calcarine (CALC), mid-cingulate cortex (MCC), and cerebellum (Kang et al., 2012). During successful inhibition, HC>OCD results were found in the IFG, hippocampus, MCC, and precuneus (Thorsen et al., 2020). During failed inhibition, HC>OCD results were found in the dmPFC, IFG, operculum and white matter (Thorsen et al., 2020). Their study also examined group differences in right and left amygdala connectivity during successful inhibition. OCD>HC results were found in right amygdala connectivity with pre-SMA, IFG, MePFC, MTG, MOG, CN, and putamen. Left amygdala connectivity was observed in MePFC, IFG, calcarine sulcus and white matter (Thorsen et al., 2020). HC>OCD were only found in right amygdala connectivity with hippocampus (tail), precuneus, and white matter (Thorsen et al., 2020).
Comparatively, Wierenga et al. (2014) aimed to analyse executive functioning in anorexia nervosa during the stop-signal task. Neuroimaging results demonstrate reduced neural activation in AN adolescents compared to healthy adolescents in many brain regions including the ACC, MFG, and PCC, with patients not presenting post-error slowing (Wierenga et al., 2014).
Only two studies (out of 32 included studies) were found to apply the WCST to measure executive functioning in OCD patients. Analyses reveal that OCD patients exhibit hyperactivation in various brain regions compared to controls. Many activations were reported, spanning across the frontal, parietal and occipital regions known for sensorimotor and cognitive processing (Kim et al., 2021). Comparatively, the PRCU, ANG, IFG have been implicated in both OCD and AN patients. Both PRCU and ANG have been shown to be hyperactivated in both patient groups when compared to controls (Kim et al., 2021; Lao-Kim et al., 2015). The IFG revealed inverse activation patterns, with OCD showing hyperactivation and AN showing hypoactivation when compared to controls (Kim et al., 2021; Sato et al., 2013). Interestingly, a recent study has reported no significant between-group differences amongst AN, OCD and HC adolescents in brain activation during WCST (Bohon et al., 2019).
Findings reveal common hyperactivation of the PFC, STG, PRCU in OCD compared to healthy controls (De Vries et al., 2013; Heinzel et al., 2017; Meram et al., 2020; Nakao et al., 2008; Vanderwee et al., 2003). Moreover, Nakao et al.’s (2008) study revealed hyperactivation of the DLPFC, insula, and cuneus. With regards to heightened activity in healthy controls compared to OCD patients, only two studies (out of six) have reported significant results (Heinzel et al., 2017; Koch et al., 2012). Such results were observed in regions related to visual, motor and emotional processing such as SMA, IOL, PoCG, CN, and amygdala (Heinzel et al., 2017). Interestingly, no significant between-group differences were found between AN and HC (Lao-Kaim et al., 2013).
Distinct brain activations have been reported during task-switching in OCD. The PoCG was shown to be hyperactive in OCD compared to HC, according to Remijnse et al. (2013) and
Liu et al. (2023). Remijnse et al. (2013) have reported more activation in the ACC and putamen and less activation in the anterior prefrontal cortex (APFC). Although Liu et al. (2023) found no hypoactivation in OCD, they did report hyperactivation in the precuneus, middle occipital cortex (MOC), middle cingulate gyrus (MCG), and superior parietal cortex (SPC). Moreover, more brain activity in HC compared to OCD was shown in the following areas: middle temporal gyrus (MTG), STG, occipital lobe (OL), ACC (rostral and dorsal), PCC, MeFC, VLPFC, PMC, CN, OFC, parietal lobe (PL), uncus, thalamus and hippocampus (Gu et al., 2007; Han et al., 2011). With regards to OCD, patients have shown increased activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and the orbitofrontal cortex during task-switch tasks (Gu et al., 2007). Hyperactivation was reported in the SPL and insula, which was associated with slower reaction times (Liu et al., 2023). This suggests the need for more brain activity to participate in the task.
During set-shifting, frontal, parietal, and occipital subregions have mainly been reported. For instance, AN patients have exhibited more activation in parietal subregions such as the precuneus, IPL, SPL, SMG (Van Autreve et al., 2016); occipital subregions such as the cuneus and calcarine cortex (De Vries et al., 2013); and in other areas like the insula and dorsal-premotor cortex (PMD; Van Autreve et al., 2016). With regards to less brain activation in AN compared to HC, occipital subregions such as inferior occipital (IO), middle occipital (MO), calcarine, lateral fusiform and lingual gyrus; frontal subregions such as the MFG, IFG, and PCG; and other regions such as the cerebellum have been reported (Castro-Fornieles et al., 2019).
Through the Tower of London task (TOL), van den Heuvel et al. (2005) have reported greater brain activation in OCD in the following areas: VLPFC, anterior temporal cortex (ATC), cingulate cortex, PHG, and brainstem. Oppositely, hypoactivation was shown in DLPFC, MFC, precuneus, PMC, IPC, CN, and putamen (Vaghi et al., 2017; Van Den Heuvel et al., 2005). Also, Kim et al. (2020) have reported reduced functional connectivity between DAN and DMN and LFPN networks in OCD.
The following studies have used other executive function tasks to report brain activity in OCD. Differently, Koçak et al. (2011) have applied an Imagination, Suppressing, and Earsing task to measure executive function-related brain activity in OCD. Overall, healthy controls have exhibited more brain activation in the PCC, SFG, and IPL compared to OCD (Koçak et al., 2011). No hyperactivation in OCD was reported.
Conversely, the opposite effect was found during an item-recognition task, with OCD presenting increased brain activation over HC. This increased brain activation was specific to the precentral sulcus, IPC, IFG, and IFS (Henseler et al., 2008).
During the Flanker task, differential activation patterns in neighbouring subregions were reported. For instance, the rostral and subgenual part of the ACC was more active in OCD relative to HC, in contrast to the dorsal part of the ACC, where HC showed more active relative to OCD (Fitzgerald et al., 2005; Grützmann et al., 2014). Other brain areas that displayed OCD>HC include the MeTG, anterior insula (AIC), SFG, IFG, SMA and amygdala (Fitzgerald et al., 2005; Grützmann et al., 2014). Brain areas that exhibited HC>OCD include the pre-SMA, SMA, and STG (Fitzgerald et al., 2005).
In Cocchi et al.’s (2011) study, they have applied the Flanker task, along with the Stroop and Simon tasks and have reported brain regions that were commonly activated during these tasks. Essentially, they reported greater brain activation in OCD relative to HC in brain regions involved in the paralimbic and sensorimotor networks. These regions include the anterior insular, pre-SMA, SMA, sensorimotor cortex, and primary motor cortex (Cocchi et al., 2011). Mainly brain regions included in the salience network and default mode network were reported to be more activated in HC relative to OCD. These regions include the MeFC, dPFC, dACC, and MCC (Cocchi et al., 2011).
With regards to AN-focused studies, studies that have applied the target-detection task and the embedded figures task have not reported any increased brain activity in AN (Leslie et al., 2021; Zastrow et al., 2009). During target-detection, the following brain areas were reported for HC>ED: MeFG, MFG, ACC, PoCG, PCG, thalamus, subthalamic nucleus, putamen, globus pallidum, and cerebellum (Zastrow et al., 2009). During an implicit sequence learning task, only between-group differences were found in the thalamus, with more activation shown in HC than AN (Firk et al., 2015).
The aim of this systematic review was to examine executive function-related brain activity in anorexia nervosa and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Throughout research, both disorders have shown executive dysfunction, which has been used to explain the manifest clinical characteristics (Del Casale et al., 2015; Diaz-Marsa et al., 2022; Kuelz et al., 2003; Martínez-Esparza et al., 2021). There are many notions that there is a possibility of shared neural mechanisms due to shared symptomatology between the two (Altman & Shankman, 2009; Gershkovich et al., 2017; Hoffman et al., 2012). The results of this review reveal distinctive neural mechanisms for each disorder. This is consistent with Thomas et al.’s (2022) review, in which AN and OCD may be utilizing different neural pathways for each executive function domain. They highlight that despite both presenting similar behavioural impairments, the way their brain adapts or compensates for executive dysfunction is different. Li et al.’s (2023) meta-analysis has also shown that both disorders present disorder-specific brain activation during inhibition.
The current review has found distinct brain activity when comparing the research based on tasks. Contrasting brain activation was observed when looking at within-group comparisons for the same tasks. For instance, research that used the go/no-go task to measure executive functioning in OCD have reported obverse results in areas such as the ACC, CN, and thalamus. Between-group comparisons for tasks were almost implausible due to research utilizing diverse tasks for each disorder.
When looking at neural activity during the n-back task, most studies have shown agreeable results regarding the correlation between task difficulty or working memory load and brain activation. For instance, during easier trials of the n-back task, OCD patients have shown hyperactivation of many brain areas such as the dACC, SMA, and IPL (De Vries et al., 2013; Koch et al., 2012; Vanderwee et al., 2003). However, this hyperactivation reduces during harder levels, revealing notions that these brain areas do not adjust well to changes in task difficulty (De Vries et al., 2013; Heinzel et al., 2017; Koch et al., 2012).
Many studies have suggested the notion of a compensatory mechanism during prolonged hyperactivation of frontoparietal areas (De Vries et al., 2013). For example, van der Wee et al.
(2003) have reported hyperactivation in the ACC during all n-back task levels, suggesting a possibility of compensation due to disturbed executive functioning other than working memory. Similar suggestions have been proposed by De Vries et al., (2013) in the DLPFC, pre-SMA, and precuneus. Interestingly, Koch et al. (2012) have reported a non-linear association between task difficulty and activation in the ACC (particularly in the dorsal area). This contrast with van der Wee et al.’s (2003) study could be due to differences in the nature of the n-back task (i.e., spatial or verbal).
Studies assessing task-switch or set shifting have noted a disintegration or conflict between internal and external models. In anorexia nervosa, higher activation in the precuneus, cuneus, lingual gyrus and insula during switching tasks is explained by higher self-referential processing and internal sensations, and less attention on external stimuli (Castro-Fornieles et al., 2019; Van Autreve et al., 2016). Similarly, reduced anticorrelation between the FPN and DMN in OCD is explained as patients' inability to disengage from internal thoughts during a task (Liu et al., 2023).
Through the Tower of London task (TOL), studies have suggested the notion of shifting from goal-directed planning to habitual responses. Vaghi et al. (2017) have reported reduced activation of the DLPFC, with weaker connections with putamen during the Tower of London task. This could suggest failure of the top-down control and could potentially explain the change from goal-directed planning to habitual responses. Furthermore, van den Heuvel et al.’s (2005) study has shown increased brain activity in the VLPFC, ACC, PHC, ATC, and brainstem, particularly as the task difficulty increases.
Moreover, studies have reported a disconnection between initial learning and cognitive flexibility. Particularly, AN patients have shown hyperactivation in the precuneus during the first trial of a switching task, suggesting that patients are able to learn new associations quickly (Lao- Kim et al., 2015). However, this pattern reduces in the second trial. Lao-Kaim et al. (2015) reported hypoactivation in the caudate nucleus after learning a new rule, suggesting a preservation error. This is further expanded through Sato et al.’s (2013) study, in which patients showed hypoactivation in the parahippocampal cortex and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. This may suggest reduced ability to plan future events.
Additionally, studies report that OCD may present an over-active error-monitoring system and generate a greater affective response to errors or conflicting stimuli. Of particular interest, hyperactivation of the ACC has been reported by three studies. Schlösser et al. (2010) particularly noticed increased activation in the dorsal part of the ACC. Fitzgerald et al. (2005) and Maltby et al. (2004) have reported higher activation in the rostral part of the ACC, suggesting that OCD patients posit high sensitivity to errors and generate greater affective responses. Also, Maltby et al. (2004) have noted that higher activation in the posterior cingulate could offer two explanations: OCD patients perceive conflict or error monitoring tasks to be highly emotional or that there is a discrepancy between expected and predicted outcomes, resulting in higher prediction errors.
Contrastingly, Wierenga et al. (2014) and Suttkus et al. (2021) have reported a hypoactive error-processing control and emotional system during go/no-go and stop-signal tasks in anorexia nervosa. Particularly, hypoactivation was shown in the MFG and PCC, suggesting that patients do not monitor their mistakes (Wierenga et al., 2014). Also, patients have shown less activation in the amygdala and hippocampus compared to healthy controls, suggesting that patients are less emotionally sensitive to errors (Suttkus et al., 2021). Reduced error-monitoring in AN could be explained by an overall diminished activity of the cognitive control system. Research has proposed
that altered activation of ACC during inhibition and altered error-monitoring could explain that patients with AN may engage in self-control processes that perpetually subside receptivity to errors (Geisler et al., 2017).
Most reported brain region among all studies was the ACC. Previous research has proposed that the ACC, specifically the dorsal area, plays a core role in inhibitory cognitive control (Salehinejad et al., 2021). Propositions have also suggested that activation of the ACC is required when elevated level of control and conflict information processing is needed (Braver et al., 2001). That is, the ACC signals the conflict information processing to other regions involved in the cognitive control network; thereafter, these brain regions activate task-relevant information and inhibit task-irrelevant information (Botvinick et al., 2001; Braver et al., 2001). Moreover, it is postulated that the dACC is involved in the consolidation of reinforcement history, goal-driven efficiency and aversive anticipation (Andrzejewski et al., 2019; Botvinick et al., 2001; Liu et al., 2024; Shenhav et al., 2013; Shenhav et al., 2016; Yu & Desrivières, 2023). Cingulate abnormalities in OCD suggests that patients have excessive or disproportionate amount of error or conflicting monitoring (Zhao et al., 2023). Zhao et al., (2023) explain OCD as misattribution of pertinence to neutral or harmless stimuli due to disruptions in the salience network (particularly the dACC and anterior insula). When looking at cingulate-subcortical circuits, it has been suggested that OCD is a result of overreliance of the direct pathway. In other words, the direct pathway is known for supporting and enacting certain behaviours; whereas the indirect pathway is known for inhibiting the direct pathway during moments when changing or switching a behaviour is needed (Salehinejad et al., 2021). Hence, accounts denote OCD to be a disorder of arbitration (Lee et al., 2014; Robbins, 2024).
In respect to preliminary research analysing ACC activation in AN, inferences posit an aberrant self-control and self-referential system (Lee et al., 2013; Northoff & Bermpohl, 2004; Northoff et al., 2006). For instance, synchronous activity between the dACC and the retrosplenial cortex in AN signifies heightened introspective cognition (Lee et al., 2013). Such results propose a crucial role of the dACC in the cognitive control of appetite and body image. Suggestions that AN can be characterized as a disorder of excessive self-control and suppression of instinctive processes were given (Lee et al., 2013). This is further expanded through the connectivity between the ventral striatum, which is associated with value assignment and prediction error; the vmPFC, which is associated with the conciliation of subjective value and learning; and the dACC, which is associated with adaptive goal-directed behaviour and erroneous actions (Lee et al., 2013; Northoff & Bermpohl, 2004). Accordingly, hypoactivation in the ACC during inhibition could suggest diminished ability towards adaptive control, detecting erroneous or conflicting cues, and weighing reinforcement history and value assignment functionally (Lee et al., 2013).
The evidence gathered from the current review has several limitations. The first limitation is the lack of consistent experimental paradigm in assessing executive functioning. Such limitation has been mentioned in prior reviews (Thomas et al., 2022). Therefore, it is not a surprise that the studies would report different brain area activations. Moreover, the inclusion of studies published in English only and with most studies using WEIRD populations could potentially lead to missed insights and overgeneralization of results. Also, only one study has assessed the difference between AN and OCD patients concurrently, with the rest of the studies have focused on only one population (Bohon et al., 2019). Interestingly, this study has reported no significant neural difference between AN, OCD and HC groups (Bohon et al., 2019). This could either be interpreted that there truly is no difference between these populations or that it is due to the task used in the
study. Therefore, more research is required to assess all groups concurrently with the same task; rather than trying to compare results based on studies who have adopted different experimental methodologies.
The current review has highlighted the need for an exhaustive delineation of the neural mechanisms underlying executive functioning tasks in AN and OCD. Although between-group comparisons on neural activity were difficult, the current review reveals a common profile of reduced integration/consensus between internal and external world models and increased self- referential processing. Such results push forward research towards assessing executive function, its relation to reinforcement learning and whether AN and OCD differ in this context. Perhaps applying methods of predictive coding or active inference (Parr et al., 2022; Redish & Gordon, 2022) could offer new insights on how executive function is truly represented in the brain for both groups.
ACC: anterior cingulate cortex AIC: anterior insula cortex ANG: angular gyrus
AN: Anorexia Nervosa
APFC: anterior prefrontal cortex ATC: anterior temporal cortex CALC: calcarine fissure/cortex CN: caudate nucleus
DAN: dorsal attention network DMN: default mode network
DLPFC: dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dPFC: dorsal prefrontal cortex
dACC: dorsal anterior cingulate cortex
fMRI: Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging FFC: fusiform face area
IFG: inferior frontal gyrus IFS: inferior frontal sulcus IPL: inferior parietal lobule IPC: inferior parietal cortex
IPJ: inferior parietal junction IOL: inferior occipital lobule LG: lingual gyrus
LFPN: left frontoparietal network MCG/C: mid-cingulate gyrus/cortex MFG/C: middle frontal gyrus/cortex MeFC: medial frontal cortex MePFC: medial prefrontal cortex MeTG: medial temporal gyrus
MOG/C: middle occipital gyrus/cortex MO: middle occipital
MTG/C: middle temporal gyrus/cortex PMC: premotor cortex
PMD: dorsal premotor cortex PM: premotor area
PCG/C: posterior cingulate gyrus/cortex PHG: parahippocampal gyrus
PoCG: postcentral gyrus PRCU: precuneus
PRG: precentral gyrus
Pre-SMA: pre-supplementary motor area PFC: prefrontal cortex
SFG: superior frontal gyrus
SMG/A: supplementary motor gyrus/area SPC: superior parietal cortex
SPL: superior parietal lobule
STG/C: superior temporal gyrus/cortex STJ: superior temporal junction VLPFC: ventrolateral prefrontal cortex WCST: Wisconsin Card Sorting Task
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Not applicable.
Not applicable.
Not applicable.
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
No funding.
Both M.S and A.A screened and evaluated all included studies used in the review and M.S was a major contributor in writing the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Not applicable.
Altman, S. E., & Shankman, S. A. (2009). What is the association between obsessive–compulsive disorder and eating disorders? Clinical Psychology Review, 29(7), 638–646. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.08.001
Andrzejewski, J. A., Greenberg, T., & Carlson, J. M. (2019). Neural correlates of aversive anticipation: An activation likelihood estimate meta-analysis across multiple sensory modalities. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(6), 1379–1390. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00747-7
Assem, M., Shashidhara, S., Glasser, M. F., & Duncan, J. (2024). Basis of executive functions in fine-grained architecture of cortical and subcortical human brain networks. Cerebral Cortex, 34(2). https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhad537
Barkley, R. A. (2001). The executive functions and self-regulation: an evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 11(1), 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1009085417776
Bohon, C., Weinbach, N., & Lock, J. (2019). Performance and brain activity during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in adolescents with obsessive–compulsive disorder and adolescents with weight-restored anorexia nervosa. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(2), 217–
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624–652. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.624
Braver, T. S., Deanna, M., Gray, J. R., Molfese, D. L., & Snyder, A. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cerebral Cortex, 11(9), 825–836. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/11.9.825
Cáceda, R., Nemeroff, C. B., & Harvey, P. D. (2014). Toward an understanding of decision making in severe mental illness. Journal of Neuropsychiatry, 26(3), 196–213. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.12110268
Castro-Fornieles, J., De La Serna, E., Calvo, A., Blázquez, A., Moya, J., Lázaro, L., Andrés- Perpiñá, S., Plana, M. T., Gil, L., Flamarique, I., Martínez, E., Pariente, J., Moreno, E., & Bargallo, N. (2019). Functional MRI with a set-shifting task in adolescent anorexia nervosa: A cross-sectional and follow-up study. Neuropsychologia, 131, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.05.019
Chan, R., Shum, D., Toulopoulou, T., & Chen, E. (2007). Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23(2), 201–216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010
Cocchi, L., Harrison, B. J., Pujol, J., Harding, I. H., Fornito, A., Pantelis, C., & Yücel, M. (2011). Functional alterations of large‐scale brain networks related to cognitive control in obsessive‐compulsive disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 33(5), 1089–1106. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21270
De Vries, F. E., De Wit, S. J., Cath, D. C., Van Der Werf, Y. D., Van Der Borden, V., Van Rossum,
De Wit, S. J., De Vries, F. E., Van Der Werf, Y. D., Cath, D. C., Heslenfeld, D. J., Veltman, E. M., Van Balkom, A. J., Veltman, D. J., & Van Den Heuvel, O. A. (2012). Presupplementary motor area hyperactivity during response inhibition: a candidate endophenotype of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(10), 1100–1108. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010073
Del Casale, A., Rapinesi, C., Kotzalidis, G. D., De Rossi, P., Curto, M., Janiri, D., Criscuolo, S., Alessi, M. C., Ferri, V. R., De Giorgi, R., Sani, G., Ferracuti, S., Girardi, P., & Brugnoli,
estimate meta-analysis of fMRI studies. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 17(5), 378–393. https://doi.org/10.3109/15622975.2015.1102323
Diaz-Marsa, M., Pemau, A., De La Torre-Luque, A., Vaz-Leal, F., Rojo-Moreno, L., Beato- Fernandez, L., Graell, M., Carrasco-Diaz, A., & Carrasco, J. L. (2022). Executive dysfunction in eating disorders: Relationship with clinical features. Progress in Neuro- Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 120, 110649.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2022.110649
Doll, B. B., Simon, D. A., & Daw, N. D. (2012). The ubiquity of model-based reinforcement learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 22(6), 1075–1081. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.08.003
Drakes, D. H., Fawcett, E. J., Rose, J. P., Carter-Major, J. C., & Fawcett, J. M. (2021). Comorbid obsessive-compulsive disorder in individuals with eating disorders: An epidemiological meta-analysis. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 141, 176–191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.06.035
Firk, C., Mainz, V., Schulte‐Ruether, M., Fink, G., Herpertz‐Dahlmann, B., & Konrad, K. (2015). Implicit sequence learning in juvenile anorexia nervosa: neural mechanisms and the impact of starvation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(11), 1168–1176. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12384
Fitzgerald, K. D., Welsh, R. C., Gehring, W. J., Abelson, J. L., Himle, J. A., Liberzon, I., & Taylor,
Fuglset, T. S., Landrø, N. I., Reas, D. L., & Rø, Ø. (2016). Functional brain alterations in anorexia nervosa: a scoping review. Journal of Eating Disorders, 4(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-016-0118-y
Geisler, D., Ritschel, F., King, J. A., Bernardoni, F., Seidel, M., Boehm, I., Runge, F., Goschke, T., Roessner, V., Smolka, M. N., & Ehrlich, S. (2017). Increased anterior cingulate cortex response precedes behavioural adaptation in anorexia nervosa. Scientific Reports, 7(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/srep42066
Gershkovich, M., Pascucci, O., & Steinglass, J. (2017). Eating Disorder Comorbidity with OCD. In Oxford University Press eBooks. https://doi.org/10.1093/med/9780190228163.003.0056
Gillan, C. M., Kosinski, M., Whelan, R., Phelps, E. A., & Daw, N. D. (2016). Characterizing a psychiatric symptom dimension related to deficits in goal-directed control. eLife, 5. https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.11305
Gillan, C. M., Robbins, T. W., Sahakian, B. J., Van Den Heuvel, O. A., & Van Wingen, G. (2015). The role of habit in compulsivity. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 26(5), 828–840. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.12.033
Gradin, V. B., Kumar, P., Waiter, G., Ahearn, T., Stickle, C., Milders, M., Reid, I., Hall, J., & Steele, J. D. (2011). Expected value and prediction error abnormalities in depression and schizophrenia. Brain, 134(6), 1751–1764. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awr059
Griffiths, K. R., Morris, R. W., & Balleine, B. W. (2014). Translational studies of goal-directed action as a framework for classifying deficits across psychiatric disorders. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00101
Gruner, P., & Pittenger, C. (2016). Cognitive inflexibility in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder.
Neuroscience, 345, 243–255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.07.030
Grützmann, R., Endrass, T., Kaufmann, C., Allen, E., Eichele, T., & Kathmann, N. (2014). Presupplementary motor area contributes to altered error monitoring in Obsessive- Compulsive Disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 80(7), 562–571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.010
Gu, B., Park, J., Kang, D., Lee, S. J., Yoo, S. Y., Jo, H. J., Choi, C., Lee, J., & Kwon, J. S. (2007).
Neural correlates of cognitive inflexibility during task-switching in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain, 131(1), 155–164. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm277
Han, J. Y., Kang, D., Gu, B., Jung, W. H., Choi, J., Choi, C., Jang, J. H., & Kwon, J. S. (2011).
Altered brain activation in ventral Frontal-Striatal regions following a 16-week pharmacotherapy in unmedicated Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Journal of Korean Medical Science, 26(5), 665. https://doi.org/10.3346/jkms.2011.26.5.665
Hawkins, J., & Blakeslee, S. (2007). On intelligence: How a New Understanding of the Brain Will Lead to the Creation of Truly Intelligent Machines. Macmillan.
Heinzel, S., Kaufmann, C., Grützmann, R., Hummel, R., Klawohn, J., Riesel, A., Bey, K., Lennertz, L., Wagner, M., & Kathmann, N. (2017). Neural correlates of working memory deficits and associations to response inhibition in obsessive compulsive disorder. NeuroImage Clinical, 17, 426–434. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2017.10.039
Henseler, I., Gruber, O., Kraft, S., Krick, C., Reith, W., & Falkai, P. (2008). Compensatory hyperactivations as markers of latent working memory dysfunctions in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder: an fMRI study. PubMed. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18592040
Hoffman, E. R., Gagne, D. A., Thornton, L. M., Klump, K. L., Brandt, H., Crawford, S., Fichter,
20(3). https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2161
Hough, C. M., Luks, T. L., Lai, K., Vigil, O., Guillory, S., Nongpiur, A., Fekri, S. M., Kupferman, E., Mathalon, D. H., & Mathews, C. A. (2016). Comparison of brain activation patterns during executive function tasks in hoarding disorder and non-hoarding OCD. Psychiatry Research Neuroimaging, 255, 50–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.07.007
Jurado, M. B., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The Elusive Nature of Executive Functions: A Review of our Current Understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17(3), 213–233. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z
Kang, D., Jang, J. H., Han, J. Y., Kim, J., Jung, W. H., Choi, J., Choi, C., & Kwon, J. S. (2012).
Neural correlates of altered response inhibition and dysfunctional connectivity at rest in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 40, 340–346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.11.001
Kappou, K., Ntougia, M., Kourtesi, A., Panagouli, E., Vlachopapadopoulou, E., Michalacos, S., Gonidakis, F., Mastorakos, G., Psaltopoulou, T., Tsolia, M., Bacopoulou, F., Sergentanis,
Kim, M., Jung, W. H., Shim, G., & Kwon, J. S. (2020). The effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors on brain functional networks during goal-directed planning in obsessive– compulsive disorder. Scientific Reports, 10(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-77814- 4
Kim, S., Lee, S. W., Cha, H., Kim, E., Chang, Y., & Lee, S. J. (2021). Reconciliation of two Cognitive Models in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: an FMRI study. Psychiatry Investigation, 18(6), 545–552. https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2020.0347
Kmet, L. M., Lee, R. C., & Research, A. H. F. F. M. (2004). Standard Quality Assessment Criteria for Evaluating Primary Research Papers from a Variety of Fields.
Koçak, O. M., Özpolat, A. Y., Atbaşoğlu, C., & Çiçek, M. (2011). Cognitive control of a simple mental image in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Brain and Cognition, 76(3), 390–399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.020
Koch, K., Wagner, G., Schachtzabel, C., Peikert, G., Schultz, C. C., Sauer, H., & Schlösser, R. G. (2012). Aberrant anterior cingulate activation in obsessive–compulsive disorder is related to task complexity. Neuropsychologia, 50(5), 958–964. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.02.002
Kopp, B. (2012). A simple hypothesis of executive function. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
Kuelz, A. K., Hohagen, F., & Voderholzer, U. (2003). Neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a critical review. Biological Psychology, 65(3), 185–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2003.07.007
Lao-Kaim, N., Giampietro, V., Williams, S., Simmons, A., & Tchanturia, K. (2013). Functional MRI investigation of verbal working memory in adults with anorexia nervosa. European Psychiatry, 29(4), 211–218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2013.05.003
Lao-Kaim, N. P., Fonville, L., Giampietro, V. P., Williams, S. C. R., Simmons, A., & Tchanturia,
Laposa, J. M., Hawley, L. L., Grimm, K. J., Katz, D. E., & Rector, N. A. (2018). What drives OCD symptom change during CBT treatment? Temporal relationships among obsessions and compulsions. Behavior Therapy, 50(1), 87–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2018.03.012
Latif, N., & Moulding, R. (2024). Lose yourself: feared self, emotion regulation, and obsessive- compulsive and eating disorder symptomatology. Australian Psychologist, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/00050067.2024.2342808
Lee, S., Kim, K. R., Ku, J., Lee, J., Namkoong, K., & Jung, Y. (2013). Resting-state synchrony between anterior cingulate cortex and precuneus relates to body shape concern in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Psychiatry Research Neuroimaging, 221(1), 43–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.11.004
Lee, S. W., Shimojo, S., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2014). Neural Computations Underlying Arbitration between Model-Based and Model-free Learning. Neuron, 81(3), 687–699. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.028
Leslie, M., Halls, D., Leppanen, J., Sedgewick, F., Lang, K., Fonville, L., Simic, M., Mandy, W., Nicholls, D., Williams, S., & Tchanturia, K. (2021). The neural correlates of a central coherence task in young women with anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review, 29(5), 744–755. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2852
Li, Z., Tong, G., Wang, Y., Ruan, H., Zheng, Z., Cheng, J., & Wang, Z. (2023). Task fMRI studies investigating inhibitory control in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and eating disorders: A comparative meta-analysis. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 25(1), 26–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/15622975.2023.2251057
Liu, Q., Gao, F., Wang, X., Xia, J., Yuan, G., Zheng, S., Zhong, M., & Zhu, X. (2023). Cognitive inflexibility is linked to abnormal frontoparietal‐related activation and connectivity in obsessive‐compulsive disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 44(16), 5460–5470. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.26457
Liu, X., Jiao, G., Zhou, F., Kendrick, K. M., Yao, D., Gong, Q., Xiang, S., Jia, T., Zhang, X., Zhang, J., Feng, J., & Becker, B. (2024). A neural signature for the subjective experience of threat anticipation under uncertainty. Nature Communications, 15(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-45433-6
Lock, J., Garrett, A., Beenhakker, J., & Reiss, A. L. (2010). Aberrant brain activation during a response inhibition task in adolescent eating disorder subtypes. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(1), 55–64. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10010056
Löffler, C., Frischkorn, G. T., Hagemann, D., Sadus, K., & Schubert, A. (2024). The common factor of executive functions measures nothing but speed of information uptake. Psychological Research, 88(4), 1092–1114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-023-01924-7
Maltby, N., Tolin, D. F., Worhunsky, P., O’Keefe, T. M., & Kiehl, K. A. (2004). Dysfunctional action monitoring hyperactivates frontal–striatal circuits in obsessive–compulsive disorder: an event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 24(2), 495–503. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.041
Mandelli, L., Draghetti, S., Albert, U., De Ronchi, D., & Atti, A. (2020). Rates of comorbid obsessive-compulsive disorder in eating disorders: A meta-analysis of the literature. Journal of Affective Disorders, 277, 927–939. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.09.003
Martínez-Esparza, I. C., Olivares-Olivares, P. J., Rosa-Alcázar, Á., Rosa-Alcázar, A. I., & Storch,
https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11020267
Masharipov, R., Korotkov, A., Knyazeva, I., Cherednichenko, D., & Kireev, M. (2023). Impaired Non-Selective Response inhibition in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(2), 1171.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20021171
Matsunaga, H., Kiriike, N., Iwasaki, Y., Miyata, A., Yamagami, S., & Kaye, W. H. (1999). Clinical characteristics in patients with anorexia nervosa and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychological Medicine, 29(2), 407–414. https://doi.org/10.1017/s003329179800796x
Meram, T. D., Chowdury, A., Easter, P., Attisha, T., Kallabat, E., Hanna, G. L., Arnold, P., Rosenberg, D. R., & Diwadkar, V. A. (2020). Evoking network profiles of the dorsal anterior cingulate in youth with Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder during motor control and working memory. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 132, 72–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.09.028
Morein-Zamir, S., Voon, V., Dodds, C. M., Sule, A., Van Niekerk, J., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins,
Nakao, T., Nakagawa, A., Nakatani, E., Nabeyama, M., Sanematsu, H., Yoshiura, T., Togao, O., Tomita, M., Masuda, Y., Yoshioka, K., Kuroki, T., & Kanba, S. (2008). Working memory dysfunction in obsessive–compulsive disorder: A neuropsychological and functional MRI study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43(8), 784–791. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.10.013
Nakao, T., Okada, K., & Kanba, S. (2014). Neurobiological model of obsessive–compulsive disorder: Evidence from recent neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 68(8), 587–605. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12195
Noda, T., Isobe, M., Ueda, K., Aso, T., Murao, E., Kawabata, M., Noma, S., & Murai, T. (2021). The relationship between attention and avoidance coping in anorexia nervosa: functional magnetic resonance imaging study. BJPsych Open, 7(4). https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2021.963
Northoff, G., & Bermpohl, F. (2004). Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(3), 102–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.004
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., De Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage, 31(1), 440–457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002
Page, L. A., Rubia, K., Deeley, Q., Daly, E., Toal, F., Mataix-Cols, D., Giampietro, V., Schmitz, N., & Murphy, D. G. (2009). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of inhibitory control in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research Neuroimaging, 174(3), 202–209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.05.002
Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., Shamseer, L., Tetzlaff, J. M., Akl, E. A., Brennan, S. E., Chou, R., Glanville, J., Grimshaw,
Parr, T., Holmes, E., Friston, K. J., & Pezzulo, G. (2023). Cognitive effort and active inference.
Neuropsychologia, 184, 108562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108562
Parr, T., Pezzulo, G., & Friston, K. J. (2022). Active inference: The Free Energy Principle in Mind, Brain, and Behavior. MIT Press.
Pezzulo, G. (2012). An Active Inference view of cognitive control. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00478
Pollack, L. O., & Forbush, K. T. (2013). Why do eating disorders and obsessive–compulsive disorder co-occur? Eating Behaviors, 14(2), 211–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.01.004
Redish, A. D., & Gordon, J. A. (2022). Computational Psychiatry: New Perspectives on Mental Illness. MIT Press.
Remijnse, P. L., Van Den Heuvel, O. A., Nielen, M. M. A., Vriend, C., Hendriks, G., Hoogendijk,
Robbins, T. W. (2024). The brain’s arbitration system and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain, 147(6), 1929–1930. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awae133
Robbins, T. W., Gillan, C. M., Smith, D. G., De Wit, S., & Ersche, K. D. (2011). Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(1), 81–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.009
Roth, R. M., Saykin, A. J., Flashman, L. A., Pixley, H. S., West, J. D., & Mamourian, A. C. (2007). Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Response Inhibition in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 62(8), 901–909. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.007
Salehinejad, M. A., Ghanavati, E., Rashid, M. H. A., & Nitsche, M. A. (2021). Hot and cold executive functions in the brain: A prefrontal-cingular network. Brain and Neuroscience Advances, 5. https://doi.org/10.1177/23982128211007769
Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: A cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23(5), 571–583. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005- 7967(85)90105-6
Sato, Y., Saito, N., Utsumi, A., Aizawa, E., Shoji, T., Izumiyama, M., Mushiake, H., Hongo, M., & Fukudo, S. (2013). Neural Basis of Impaired Cognitive Flexibility in Patients with Anorexia Nervosa. PLoS ONE, 8(5), e61108. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061108
Saxena, S., O’Neill, J., & Rauch, S. L. (2009). The role of cingulate cortex dysfunction in obsessive– compulsive disorder. In Oxford University Press eBooks (pp. 587–618). https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198566960.003.0027
Schlösser, R. G., Wagner, G., Schachtzabel, C., Peikert, G., Koch, K., Reichenbach, J. R., & Sauer,
Scholl, J., & Klein-Flügge, M. (2017). Understanding psychiatric disorder by capturing ecologically relevant features of learning and decision-making. Behavioural Brain Research, 355, 56–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2017.09.050
Shafran, R. (2002). Eating disorders and obsessive compulsive disorder. In Elsevier eBooks (pp.
215–231). https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-008043410-0/50015-5
Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79(2), 217–240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007
Shenhav, A., Cohen, J. D., & Botvinick, M. M. (2016). Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the value of control. Nature Neuroscience, 19(10), 1286–1291. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4384
Sherman, B. J., Savage, C. R., Eddy, K. T., Blais, M. A., Deckersbach, T., Jackson, S. C., Franko,
Stephan, K. E., & Mathys, C. (2014). Computational approaches to psychiatry. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 25, 85–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.12.007
Sternheim, L. C., Van Passel, B., Dingemans, A., Cath, D., & Danner, U. N. (2022). Cognitive and experienced flexibility in patients with anorexia nervosa and obsessive compulsive disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.868921
Suttkus, S., Schumann, A., De La Cruz, F., & Bär, K. (2021). Attenuated neuronal and autonomic responses during error processing in anorexia nervosa. Brain and Behavior, 11(8). https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.2235
Taylor, S., Abramowitz, J. S., & McKay, D. (2007). Cognitive-Behavioral Models of Obsessive- Compulsive Disorder. In American Psychological Association eBooks (pp. 9–29). https://doi.org/10.1037/11543-001
Tchanturia, K., Davies, H., Roberts, M., Harrison, A., Nakazato, M., Schmidt, U., Treasure, J., & Morris, R. (2012). Poor Cognitive Flexibility in Eating Disorders: Examining the Evidence
using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task. PLoS ONE, 7(1), e28331. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028331
Thomas, K. S., Birch, R. E., Jones, C. R. G., & Vanderwert, R. E. (2022). Neural correlates of executive functioning in Anorexia Nervosa and Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.841633
Thorsen, A. L., De Wit, S. J., Hagland, P., Ousdal, O. T., Hansen, B., Hagen, K., Kvale, G., & Van Den Heuvel, O. A. (2020). Stable inhibition-related inferior frontal hypoactivation and fronto-limbic hyperconnectivity in obsessive–compulsive disorder after concentrated exposure therapy. NeuroImage Clinical, 28, 102460.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102460
Tolin, D. F., Witt, S. T., & Stevens, M. C. (2013). Hoarding disorder and obsessive–compulsive disorder show different patterns of neural activity during response inhibition. Psychiatry Research Neuroimaging, 221(2), 142–148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.11.009
Vaghi, M. M., Hampshire, A., Fineberg, N. A., Kaser, M., Brühl, A. B., Sahakian, B. J., Chamberlain, S. R., & Robbins, T. W. (2017). Hypoactivation and dysconnectivity of a frontostriatal circuit during Goal-Directed planning as an endophenotype for Obsessive- Compulsive Disorder. Biological Psychiatry Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 2(8), 655–663. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.05.005
Van Autreve, S., Baene, W. D., Baeken, C., Van Heeringen, K., Vancayseele, N., & Vervaet, M. (2016). Differential neural correlates of Set‐Shifting in the Bingeing–Purging and Restrictive subtypes of anorexia nervosa: an FMRI study. European Eating Disorders Review, 24(4), 277–285. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2437
Van Den Heuvel, O. A., Veltman, D. J., Groenewegen, H. J., Cath, D. C., Van Balkom, A. J. L. M., Van Hartskamp, J., Barkhof, F., & Van Dyck, R. (2005). Frontal-Striatal dysfunction during planning in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(3), 301. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.301
Vanderwee, N., Ramsey, N., Jansma, J., Denys, D., Vanmegen, H., Westenberg, H., & Kahn, R. (2003). Spatial working memory deficits in obsessive compulsive disorder are associated with excessive engagement of the medial frontal cortex. NeuroImage, 20(4), 2271–2280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.05.001
Voon, V., Reiter, A., Sebold, M., & Groman, S. (2017). Model-Based control in dimensional psychiatry. Biological Psychiatry, 82(6), 391–400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.04.006
Wierenga, C., Bischoff-Grethe, A., Melrose, A. J., Grenesko-Stevens, E., Irvine, Z., Wagner, A., Simmons, A., Matthews, S., Yau, W. W., Fennema-Notestine, C., & Kaye, W. H. (2014). Altered BOLD Response during Inhibitory and Error Processing in Adolescents with Anorexia Nervosa. PLoS ONE, 9(3), e92017. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092017
Williams, B. M., Brown, M. L., Sandoval-Araujo, L., Russell, S., & Levinson, C. A. (2022). Psychiatric Comorbidity among Eating Disorders and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder and
Underlying Shared Mechanisms and Features: An updated review. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36(3), 226–246. https://doi.org/10.1891/jcpsy-d-2021-0011
Wollesen, B., Wanstrath, M., Van Schooten, K. S., & Delbaere, K. (2019). A taxonomy of cognitive tasks to evaluate cognitive-motor interference on spatiotemoporal gait parameters in older people: a systematic review and meta-analysis. European Review of Aging and Physical Activity, 16(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s11556-019-0218-1
Yilmaz, Z., Halvorsen, M., Bryois, J., Yu, D., Thornton, L. M., Zerwas, S., Micali, N., Moessner, R., Burton, C. L., Zai, G., Erdman, L., Kas, M. J., Arnold, P. D., Davis, L. K., Knowles, J.
A., Breen, G., Scharf, J. M., Nestadt, G., Mathews, C. A., . . . Crowley, J. J. (2018). Examination of the shared genetic basis of anorexia nervosa and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 25(9), 2036–2046. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-
Youssef, A. M., AbouHendy, W., Elshabrawy, A., & Amin, S. I. (2020). Executive function in obsessive compulsive disorder at Zagazig University Hospitals: a case-control study. Middle East Current Psychiatry, 27(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s43045-020-00033-0
Yu, X., & Desrivières, S. (2023). Altered anticipatory brain responses in eating disorders: A neuroimaging meta‐analysis. European Eating Disorders Review, 31(3), 363–376. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2967
Zastrow, A., Kaiser, S., Stippich, C., Walther, S., Herzog, W., Tchanturia, K., Belger, A., Weisbrod, M., Treasure, J., & Friederich, H. (2009). Neural correlates of impaired Cognitive-Behavioral flexibility in anorexia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(5), 608–616. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050775
Zhao, Y., Zhang, Y., Wang, Q., Manssuer, L., Cui, H., Ding, Q., Sun, B., Liu, W., & Voon, V. (2023). Evidence accumulation and neural correlates of uncertainty in Obsessive- Compulsive Disorder. Biological Psychiatry Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 8(10), 1058–1065. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2023.05.011